Negative reinforcement boosts behavior by removing discomfort.
Combining it with positive reinforcement works best.
Overuse can lead to avoidance & stress.
Often mistaken for punishment, negative reinforcement is a powerful tool for shaping behavior.
It works by removing an unpleasant stimulus to encourage a desired action, like turning off an alarm clock and getting out of bed.
From parenting and workplace motivation to animal training and everyday habits, negative reinforcement influences our decisions more than we realize.
But is it always effective? And how does it compare to positive reinforcement? In this article, we’ll break down the science, explore real-life examples, and uncover when and how negative reinforcement can be a game-changer or a slippery slope.
Before you continue, we thought you might like to download our three Positive Education Exercises for free. These ready-made tools are perfect for enhancing your teaching approach, making it easier to engage students in meaningful, student-centered learning.
Negative reinforcement, a concept introduced by B. F. Skinner’s (1938) operant conditioning theory, involves increasing the likelihood of a behavior by eliminating an aversive or undesirable stimulus following the desired response.
Unlike punishment, which decreases behavior, negative reinforcement encourages the repetition of behavior by eliminating discomfort or aversive conditions (Skinner, 1938).
The Science Behind It
Negative reinforcement, where behaviors are strengthened by removing something unpleasant, is a powerful concept in operant conditioning.
For example, if a person takes pain medication to relieve a headache, removing pain reinforces the act of taking medication, making it more likely that they will take medication again in the future (Bouton, 2007).
Neurologically, the brain responds to relief by reinforcing actions that prevent discomfort. This is also seen in avoidance learning, which is also a type of negative reinforcement in which people learn to dodge negative outcomes, like finishing work early to avoid stress.
Whether in daily habits or major life decisions, negative reinforcement shapes behavior by encouraging actions that bring relief.
Operant conditioning
Negative reinforcement is a key concept within operant conditioning, a learning theory developed by B. F. Skinner (1965) that explains how behaviors are strengthened or weakened based on their consequences.
Negative reinforcement occurs when behavior increases because it removes or prevents an aversive stimulus (Domjan, 2006). For more on operant conditioning, please refer to this engaging video.
Negative reinforcement involves key brain structures and neurotransmitter systems that mediate learning, motivation, and avoidance behaviors. The process is primarily regulated by the dopaminergic system, the amygdala, and the prefrontal cortex, which together influence how organisms learn from aversive experiences (Schultz, 2016).
Avoidance and example learning
Avoidance learning is a form of negative reinforcement in which a behavior is strengthened because it prevents or removes an aversive stimulus before it occurs (Mowrer, 1951). This type of learning is common in both animals and humans and plays a crucial role in adaptive and maladaptive behaviors.
Negative reinforcement and punishment are often confused, but they work in opposite ways.
Negative reinforcement strengthens behavior by removing an unpleasant stimulus, like leaving early to avoid traffic. Punishment, on the other hand, reduces behavior by adding an unpleasant consequence, such as getting a fine for speeding. Understanding the difference helps shape behavior effectively.
Pros and cons of each method
Negative reinforcement and punishment each have advantages and drawbacks when shaping behavior. Let’s explore them.
Pros of negative reinforcement
Implementing negative reinforcement strategies can effectively increase compliance with tasks and reduce problematic behaviors (Athens & Vollmer, 2010).
Negative reinforcement has been associated with positive emotional outcomes. Research indicates that participants subjected to negative reinforcement reported higher levels of positive affect and joviality, along with decreased fatigue, compared to other reinforcement types (Xu et al., 2021).
Negative reinforcement can promote the learning of new skills in educational contexts. By removing aversive stimuli contingent on correct responses, learners are motivated to acquire and demonstrate desired behaviors, thereby enhancing the learning process (Iwata, 1987).
Cons of negative reinforcement
Negative reinforcement can inadvertently encourage individuals to evade situations associated with discomfort rather than addressing the underlying issues.
Piazza et al.’s (1997) study demonstrated that when a person learns they can avoid something they dislike by misbehaving, the misbehavior becomes a learned avoidance strategy, which is negative reinforcement in action.
Behaviors maintained through negative reinforcement can be challenging to modify. Research highlights that treatments for escape-maintained behaviors require careful consideration to avoid reinforcing the undesired behavior (Iwata, 1987).
For example, a student may throw a tantrum or act out when asked to complete an assignment. If the teacher then allows the student to take a break or removes the assignment, the student will escape the task. Over time, the student may continue to use tantrums to avoid similar work.
Negative reinforcement can sometimes conflict with positive reinforcement strategies, complicating behavior modification efforts.
For example, when comparing the effects of positive and negative reinforcement, a study found that competition between these reinforcement types can impact the effectiveness of treatments for escape behavior (Lalli et al., 1999). The results suggest positive reinforcement can be a more effective strategy for escape-maintained behaviors.
Pros of punishment
Punishment can lead to a rapid decrease in unwanted behaviors. For instance, a study demonstrated that functional communication training combined with punishment was more effective in reducing problem behaviors than functional communication training alone (Hanley et al., 2005).
Some individuals may prefer interventions that include punishment components. Research indicates that participants with severe problem behaviors clearly preferred treatments of functional communication training with punishment over those without the punishment component (Hanley et al., 2005).
Cons of punishment
The use of punishment raises significant ethical and legal questions. The Senate Subcommittee on Constitutional Rights has examined the implications of behavior modification techniques, highlighting concerns about individual rights and government involvement (Whitman, 1974).
Punishment-based interventions are controversial due to the risks of psychological and behavioral issues. A national survey revealed diverse views on the efficacy and ethics of such interventions, emphasizing the need for careful consideration (Reed & Lovett, 2007).
While punishment can have immediate effects, overreliance may diminish its effectiveness over time and lead to negative outcomes. Studies suggest that excessive use of punishment can reduce autonomy and harm behavior (Chen, 2023). Further, the method may sometimes lead to fear or resentment in those receiving punishment (Bangash et al., 2024).
Negative Reinforcement vs. Positive Reinforcement
Negative reinforcement and positive reinforcement are both strategies used to encourage behavior, but they work in different ways.
Positive reinforcement strengthens behavior by adding a rewarding stimulus, like praise or a treat, while negative reinforcement strengthens behavior by removing an unpleasant stimulus, like turning off a loud alarm after completing a task (Evans, 2015).
Understanding the differences between these approaches can help in effectively shaping behavior in various settings.
Why pair them?
Studies suggest that pairing negative and positive reinforcement can enhance learning and task compliance, especially under time constraints, by creating a dynamic that is more effective than using either alone (Bouxsein et al., 2011; Wergård et al., 2015).
Real-world examples
To reiterate the effectiveness, here are some practical examples where the pairings help reinforce desired behaviors through both motivation and relief.
In the classroom, a teacher rewards a student with praise (positive reinforcement) for completing their work on time while also removing extra homework for consistent good behavior (negative reinforcement).
In the workplace, employees receive a bonus for exceeding their targets (positive reinforcement) and no longer have to attend mandatory check-in meetings after proving their efficiency (negative reinforcement).
In parenting, a child earns extra playtime for finishing their chores (positive reinforcement) while also getting out of a disliked chore, like washing dishes, for consistently keeping their room clean (negative reinforcement).
Examples of Negative Reinforcement + Practical Applications
Negative reinforcement is a powerful behavioral principle that strengthens desired behaviors by removing or avoiding an aversive stimulus.
Understanding real-life examples and practical applications can help illustrate how this concept is used in various settings, from therapy to the workplace.
In parenting
A child is whining while doing a difficult math worksheet. When they ask for help politely, the parent sits down and helps them, which stops the whining and frustration. Over time, the child learns to ask for help rather than whine. The unpleasant stimulus is disappointment and confusion. The behavior of politely asking for help immediately alleviates the frustration.
A student who studies diligently to avoid parental criticism is exhibiting behavior shaped by negative reinforcement. Skinner’s (1938) research demonstrated how this process influences learning and decision-making, emphasizing its role in habit formation and behavior modification.
In exposure therapy for anxiety disorders, a therapist may guide a client through gradual exposure to a feared situation. As the client engages in exposure exercises, their anxiety (the aversive stimulus) decreases over time, reinforcing their willingness to face the fear.
In the workplace
A manager stops micromanaging an employee after they consistently meet deadlines. For instance, if an employee has consistently submitted reports on time for several weeks, the manager may stop sending frequent reminder emails. Removing these reminders reinforces the employee’s timely submissions, increasing the likelihood of continued punctuality.
Research supports the effectiveness of negative reinforcement in behavior modification, particularly in applied settings.
For example, Iwata (1987) highlights how behaviors maintained through negative reinforcement can be adjusted by systematically removing aversive stimuli.
However, studies by Lalli et al. (1999) suggest that positive reinforcement may be more effective in reducing problem behavior. A balanced approach integrating both reinforcement types can enhance outcomes (Piazza et al., 1996). While useful, negative reinforcement requires careful application to avoid reinforcing undesired behaviors.
World’s Largest Positive Psychology Resource – Save 18% For a Limited Time.
Research has shown both benefits and drawbacks of using negative reinforcement in behavior modification across various contexts. Let’s elaborate.
Advantages
Negative reinforcement can effectively increase compliance with tasks. For instance, Athens and Vollmer (2010) found that implementing negative reinforcement strategies in the classrooms led to higher compliance rates and reduced problematic behaviors.
Negative reinforcement can lead to swift changes in behavior. Research by Lerman and Iwata (1996) demonstrated that removing an aversive stimulus immediately following a desired behavior increases the likelihood of that behavior being repeated. This phenomenon is commonly observed in educational and therapeutic settings.
In workplace environments, negative reinforcement has been shown to enhance productivity. Studies suggest that employees who receive fewer performance-related criticisms when meeting deadlines tend to maintain higher levels of task efficiency (Piazza et al., 1997).
Disadvantages
Overreliance on negative reinforcement may encourage avoidance rather than active engagement with tasks.
Research by Mowrer (1951) on avoidance learning demonstrated that individuals are likely to develop avoidance behaviors instead of directly addressing the cause of the aversive stimulus. This can hinder long-term problem-solving skills.
Behaviors maintained through negative reinforcement can be challenging to modify. Iwata (1987) highlighted that behaviors reinforced through escape or avoidance mechanisms may persist when interventions aim to replace them with more adaptive behaviors.
Excessive use of negative reinforcement can lead to increased stress and anxiety. Research indicates that individuals exposed to high levels of negative reinforcement in learning environments may develop heightened anxiety, which can negatively impact their performance (Hammond et al., 2020).
A Step-By-Step Guide for Using Negative Reinforcers Effectively
Follow these structured steps to maximize the effectiveness of negative reinforcement while minimizing unintended consequences (Huitt, 1994; Iwata, 1987; Miltenberger, 2016).
Step 1
Clearly define the behavior you want to increase. Ensure that it is measurable and observable.
Step 2
Identify the stimulus that the individual finds unpleasant and would be motivated to escape or avoid.
Step 3
Ensure that the removal of the aversive stimulus relies on the occurrence of the desired behavior.
Step 4
Introduce the negative reinforcer in a controlled manner. Avoid removing the aversive stimulus too quickly, as this may weaken the behavior change. Gradual reduction fosters long-term success.
Step 5
Observe changes in the individual’s behavior to track the effectiveness of the reinforcement strategy. If the desired behavior does not increase, reassess the aversive stimulus and reinforcement schedule.
Step 6
For the best results, consider complementing negative reinforcement with positive reinforcement. Once the behavior becomes more consistent, introduce rewards like praise, privileges, or incentives to sustain motivation.
Step 7
Relying too much on negative reinforcement can create stress and anxiety. Balance reinforcement strategies to maintain a positive, productive environment.
Top 17 Exercises for Positive Education
Use these 17 Positive Education Exercises [PDF] to enhance student engagement, resilience, and wellbeing while also equipping students with valuable life skills.
Negative reinforcement is often misunderstood, leading to several common myths that distort its role in behavior change. Clarifying these misconceptions is essential for using reinforcement strategies effectively in educational, therapeutic, and everyday settings.
1. Negative means bad or harmful
In behavioral psychology, the term “negative” refers to removing something (Cooper et al., 2007). In this context, the word negative does not imply harm or anything bad.
2. Negative reinforcement is the same as punishment
Negative reinforcement increases behavior by removing an aversive stimulus, while punishment decreases behavior (Skinner, 1965). We cannot regard them as identical because they operate differently.
3. Negative reinforcement only works with animals
Negative reinforcement can be frequently observed in various settings, including education, therapy, and real-life events (Domjan, 2006). Further, negative reinforcement is a fundamental learning process in humans, used to reduce anxiety, encourage task completion, and manage challenging behaviors (Piazza et al., 1997).
4. Negative reinforcement is unethical
Negative reinforcement is not inherently unethical. Ethics depend on the context, intention, and impact (Schwartz & Kusyk, 2017).
5. Negative reinforcement is more effective than positive reinforcement
Studies have shown that positive reinforcement not only strengthens desired behaviors but also contributes to more enduring change, making it a powerful tool in both classrooms and therapy environments (Cameron & Pierce, 1994).
A Take-Home Message
Negative reinforcement is a powerful tool for shaping behavior, but its effectiveness depends on careful application. When used strategically, it can promote desired behaviors by removing an unpleasant stimulus, reinforcing positive habits in parenting, education, therapy, and the workplace.
However, overreliance on negative reinforcement can lead to avoidance behaviors and unintended consequences.
Research suggests that combining negative and positive reinforcement may yield the best results, encouraging both motivation and long-term behavior change.
By understanding the science behind negative reinforcement and implementing it thoughtfully, individuals and organizations can create structured environments that promote learning, productivity, and overall behavioral improvement.
As you reflect on your practice, how might you use negative reinforcement more intentionally to support growth while maintaining trust and emotional safety with those you serve?
How does negative reinforcement differ from punishment?
While negative reinforcement increases a behavior by removing an unpleasant stimulus, punishment decreases a behavior by introducing an unpleasant consequence.
Can you provide examples of negative reinforcement?
Examples include taking pain medication to relieve a headache or cleaning the house to avoid nagging from a partner.
Is negative reinforcement always effective?
Negative reinforcement can be effective, but overuse may lead to avoidance behaviors and stress.
References
Athens, E. S., & Vollmer, T. R. (2010). An investigation of differential reinforcement of alternative behavior without extinction. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 43(4), 569–589. https://doi.org/10.1901/jaba.2010.43-569
Bangash, M., Nayab, T., Zubair, M., Nawaz, H., Ullah, I., & Ullah, S. (2024). Examining the impacts of corporal punishment in secondary schools: Unraveling psychological repercussions on students. Journal of Health and Rehabilitation Research, 4(2). https://doi.org/10.61919/jhrr.v4i2.828
Bouton, M. E. (2007). Learning and behavior: A contemporary synthesis. Sinauer Associates.
Bouton, M. E. (2019). Extinction of instrumental (operant) learning: Interference, varieties of context, and mechanisms of contextual control. Psychopharmacology, 236(1), 7–19. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00213-018-5076-4
Bouxsein, K., Roane, H., & Harper, T. (2011). Evaluating the separate and combined effects of positive and negative reinforcement on task compliance. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 44(1), 175–179. https://doi.org/10.1901/jaba.2011.44-175
Cameron, J., & Pierce, W. D. (1994). Reinforcement, reward, and intrinsic motivation: A meta-analysis. Review of Educational Research, 64(3), 363–423. https://doi.org/10.3102/00346543064003363
Chen, X. (2023). A study of using reward and punishment in the education of school-aged children—based on behaviorism theory operant conditioning. Journal of Education, Humanities and Social Sciences, 10, 86–90.
Cooper, J. O., Heron, T. E., & Heward, W. L. (2007). Applied behavior analysis. Pearson.
Domjan, M. (2006). The principles of learning and behavior. Wadsworth Publishing Company.
Hammond, D., Xu, P., Ai, H., & Van Dam, N. (2020). Anxiety and depression related abnormalities in socio-affective learning. Journal of Affective Disorders. https://doi.org/10.31234/osf.io/tg3ec
Hanley, G. P., Piazza, C. C., Fisher, W. W., & Maglieri, K. A. (2005). On the effectiveness of and preference for punishment and extinction components of function‐based interventions. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 38(1), 51–65. https://doi.org/10.1901/jaba.2005.6-04
Iwata, B. A. (1987). Negative reinforcement in applied behavior analysis: An emerging technology. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 20(4), 361–378. https://doi.org/10.1901/jaba.1987.20-361
Lalli, J. S., Vollmer, T. R., Progar, P. R., Wright, C., Borrero, J., Daniel, D., Bathold, C. H., Tocco, K, & May, W. (1999). Competition between positive and negative reinforcement in the treatment of escape behavior. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 32(3), 285–296. https://doi.org/10.1901/jaba.1999.32-285
Leeder, T. M. (2022). Behaviorism, Skinner, and operant conditioning: Considerations for sport coaching practice. Strategies, 35(3), 27–32. https://doi.org/10.1080/08924562.2022.2052776
Lerman, D. C., & Iwata, B. A. (1996). Developing a technology for the use of operant extinction in clinical settings: An examination of basic and applied research. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 29(3), 345–382. https://doi.org/10.1901/jaba.1996.29-345
Miltenberger, R. G. (2016). Behavior modification: Principles and procedures. Cengage Learning.
Mowrer, O. H. (1951). Two-factor learning theory: Summary and comment. Psychological Review, 58(5), 350–354. https://doi.org/10.1037/h0058956
Piazza, C. C., Fisher, W. W., Hanley, G. P., Remick, M. L., Contrucci, S. A., & Aitken, T. L. (1997). The use of positive and negative reinforcement in the treatment of escape‐maintained destructive behavior. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 30(2), 279–298. https://doi.org/10.1901/jaba.1997.30-279
Piazza, C. C., Moes, D. R., & Fisher, W. W. (1996). Differential reinforcement of alternative behavior and demand fading in the treatment of escape‐maintained destructive behavior. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 29(4), 569–572. https://doi.org/10.1901/jaba.1996.29-569
Reed, F. D. D., & Lovett, B. J. (2007). Views on the efficacy and ethics of punishment: Results from a national survey. International Journal of Behavioral Consultation and Therapy, 4(1), 61–67. https://doi.org/10.1037/h0100832
Schwartz, M., & Kusyk, S. M. (2017). Ethical decision-making theory: Revisiting the moral intensity construct. Academy of Management, 2017(1), 16266. https://doi.org/10.5465/AMBPP.2017.16266abstract
Skinner, B. F. (1938). The behavior of organisms: An experimental analysis. BF Skinner Foundation.
Skinner, B. F. (1965). Science and human behavior. Simon and Schuster.
Wergård, E., Temrin, H., Forkman, B., Spångberg, M., Fredlund, H., & Westlund, K. (2015). Training pair-housed Rhesus macaques (Macaca mulatta) using a combination of negative and positive reinforcement. Behavioural Processes, 113, 51–59. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.beproc.2014.12.008
Whitman, M. A. (1974). Behavior modification: Introduction and implications. De Paul Law Review, 24(4), 949–959.
Xu, S., Sun, Y., Huang, M., Huang, Y., Han, J., Tang, X., & Ren, W. (2021). Emotional state and feedback-related negativity induced by positive, negative, and combined reinforcement. Frontiers in Psychology, 12, 647263. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2021.647263
About the author
Tiffany Sauber Millacci, Ph.D., is an educator who works with both university and elementary students. As a lifelong learner, she is driven by a passion for research and enjoys translating her findings into writing to share her knowledge with others.